Advertisement

natureNature
clockPUBLISHED

Moth Night Vision is Specialized for Tracking Flowers Swaying in the Breeze

509 Moth Night Vision is Specialized for Tracking Flowers Swaying in the Breeze
A hawkmoth with its proboscis extended approaches a robotic flower on which another is already feeding. They slow their brains to improve vision under low-light conditions while continuing to perform demanding tasks / Rob Felt, Georgia Tech

Hawkmoths are big, agile insects that hover in place expertly as they feed on nectar at dawn, dusk, and in the evenings. So, not only is it dark out, but the flowers are moving targets thanks to the breeze. Now, with the help of a robotic flower, researchers reveal how hawkmoths forage in the darkness: Their twilight or night vision is exquisitely attuned to flowers swaying in the wind. The findings, published in Science this week, suggest that hawkmoth sight and flight evolved to perfectly match the movements of their only source of food.

"There has been a lot of interest in understanding how animals deal with challenging sensing environments, especially when they are also doing difficult tasks like hovering in mid-air," Georgia Tech’s Simon Sponberg says in a news release. It’s a feat from both a sensory and motor control perspective. 

Advertisement

The hawkmoth’s large compound eyes (which you can see below) help maximize the amount of light that’s captured. Furthermore, to compensate for dim, changing light conditions, nocturnal and crepuscular (or twilight) flying insects process light over longer times—not unlike a photographer working in low lighting. Slowing down visual processing increases light sensitivity, but it also reduces response times. "Using this temporal strategy is sort of like changing the shutter speed on a camera to increase exposure," Sponberg tells the Washington Post. "And if you do that with a series of frames, each gets brighter, but also blurrier. It gets harder to see fast things." Slowing their tiny brains improves low-light vision, but now their tracking lags behind.

So, how are these hummingbird-sized insects able to track wind-tossed flowers when it’s dark? To see, Sponberg and colleagues tempted Manduca sexta hawkmoths with a 3D-printed flower filled with nectar and attached to a robotic arm (pictured above). They used high-speed infrared cameras to monitor how well the free-flying moths kept their proboscises (or their tongues) in the nectar dispenser. Also, the flower was programmed to move side-to-side at various rates up to 20 Hertz, or 20 cycles per second. Since their wings beat at a rate of 25 strokes per second, that means they’d have to adjust their direction with almost every wingstroke. 

The team discovered that in dark, moonlit conditions, the moth’s tracking responses were 17 percent slower compared to brighter, early-dusk light. Furthermore, the hawkmoths had a hard time tracking flowers moving at frequencies higher than 1.7 Hertz. When the team analyzed the movements of five of the moth’s favorite (real) flowers as they blew in the wind, they realized that 94 percent of the flowers’ motions remained below 1.7 Hertz. 

"This was an interesting example of how an organism can tune its brain to maintain its ability to gather food," Sponberg explains. They expected moths to track poorly in low-light conditions. "The moths do suffer a trade-off by slowing their brains, but that trade-off doesn't end up mattering because it only affects their ability to track movements that don't exist in the natural way that flowers blow in the wind." 

Advertisement

 

 

Images: Rob Felt, Georgia Tech (top), Armin Hinterwirth (middle)


ARTICLE POSTED IN

natureNature
  • tag
  • flowers,

  • night vision,

  • tracking,

  • moth,

  • hawkmoths,

  • light sensitivity

FOLLOW ONNEWSGoogele News