Advertisement

clockPUBLISHED

Monkeys Capable Of Mathematics

author

Stephen Luntz

Stephen has a science degree with a major in physics, an arts degree with majors in English Literature and History and Philosophy of Science and a Graduate Diploma in Science Communication.

Freelance Writer

746 Monkeys Capable Of Mathematics
Margaret S. Livingstone. Rhesus monkeys can learn to associate symbols with quantity and that when it comes to rewards size matters
Next time you describe a job as so easy a trained monkey could do it, consider you might be underselling them. Rhesus monkeys have been found to be able to learn simple addition, and their rare errors may tell us something about how we estimate quantities ourselves.
 
Many animals have a sense of number. In his book The Mathematical Brain, Brian Butterworth describes experiments that have confirmed this across many species, such as one where lions were played the recorded roars of fellow Panthera leo. When the sounds came from fewer animals than made up the pride being tested they would set out to fight for their territory, but when the sounds suggested they were outnumbered they backed off.
 
Butterworth also has a story about a troupe of chimpanzees that communicated over large distances by hitting hollow trees, with the leader sending a messages in code – one strike meaning change direction, two for rest.
 
The capacity to add and subtract numbers together in symbolic form is a different matter however, although some past studies have suggested some primates may be capable of this too.
 
Professor Margaret Livingstone of the Harvard Medical School taught rhesus monkeys the meaning of numbers from zero to 25 using the symbols for 0-9 and 16 letters of the alphabet. The teaching was done using drops of a reward so that the larger number was associated with more drops. The monkeys were then given a choice of two symbols and given a number of drops of the reward equal to the side they chose. Consequently, it was in their interest to choose the larger side.
 
Once this had been learned successfully the monkeys were given two symbols and had to compare them with a single one. At first the monkeys were inclined to choose the side with the single number, if it was larger than either numbers on the other side on their own. With time they got better, realizing that two smaller numbers could combined be better than one larger one. Livingstone reports in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences that the animals were successful 90% of the time. Interestingly, however, the monkeys still placed more weight on the larger number than the smaller one – that is they were more likely to pick a side with a 2 and a 9, when compared with a 10, than a 4 and 7, even though both add to 11.
 
Past tests of animal intelligence have often run into trouble because certain animals reached the same conclusion through a different method from what was expected. The team worried that through prolonged practice the monkeys might have memorized all possible pairings, demonstrating an extraordinary memory rather than any capacity for calculation. So Livingstone and her colleagues gave their subjects a new set of characters and taught them what each meant. Without further prompting the monkeys started using maths to work out which combinations were larger.
 
A further intriguing insight into the monkey mind came from the observations that when the monkeys did get it wrong it was usually when the totals were close together; 6+7 was hard when compared to 12, but easy when compared to 9. Rather than having a precise calculation the mathemagicians were using estimates, which may offer insight into the way humans do the same calculations.
 
Livingstone is hoping to find the basis for Weber's Law, which says that how large the difference between two stimuli has to be for us to notice it depends not on the absolute size of the difference, but on the size relative to the magnitude of the stimuli. As the paper puts it, “Although it is easy to recognize the difference between 2 and 4 items, it is more difficult to distinguish 22 and 24 items.” The same occurs when we are trying to distinguish the size or weight of an object, or in estimating periods of time.
 
Livingstone and her co-authors say, “Weber’s law can be explained either by a compressive scaling of sensory response with stimulus magnitude or by a proportional scaling of response variability. These two mechanisms can be distinguished by asking how quantities are added or subtracted.” The authors conclude,  “The way [the monkeys] combined pairs of symbols indicated neither a linear nor a compressed scale, but rather a dynamically shifting, relative scaling.”

ARTICLE POSTED IN

FOLLOW ONNEWSGoogele News